Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28 (2004) 663–674
Memory processes in classical conditioning
Department of Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington, VT 05405, USA
Classical conditioning provides a rich and powerful method for studying basic learning, memory, and emotion processes in animals.
However, it is important to recognize that an animal’s performance in a conditioning experiment provides only an indirect indication of whatit has learned. Various remembering and forgetting processes, in addition to other psychological processes, may intervene and complicatewhat investigators can infer about learning from performance. This article reviews the role of context, interference, and retrieval in a numberof classical conditioning phenomena (e.g. extinction), and provides an overview of how long-term and short-term memory processesinfluence behavior as it is studied in classical conditioning.
q 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Learning vs. performance; Context; Extinction; Short-term memory; Long-term memory
1. Extinction, context, and interference . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 664
2. Long-term memory processes in animal conditioning and learning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 665
3. Short-term memory processes in conditioning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 668
4. Conditioned stimuli as retrieval cues . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 669
5. Summary and conclusion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 670
Acknowledgements . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 671
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 671
Classical conditioning in animals provides a powerful
Given proper control groups for alternative nonassociative
tool for studying the biological processes underlying
processes such as sensitization and pseudoconditioning, the
learning, memory, and emotion. In conditioning, once
evocation of the CR is a reasonably good index of learning.
a conditional stimulus (CS) is associated with an uncondi-
But it is important to realize that what an animal does in a
tional stimulus (US), a constellation of conditioned
conditioning experiment is not the same as what it knows.
responses (CRs) comes to be elicited by the CS.
Researchers in behavioral aspects of learning and memoryhave long separated learning, the hypothetical psychologi-cal and physical changes in the brain, from performance,
* Corresponding author. Tel.: C1 802 656 4164; fax: C1 802 656 8783.
the manifestation of that change in behavior. The present
E-mail addresses: mark.bouton@uvm.edu (M.E. Bouton), mbouton@
article selectively reviews the kinds of memory processes
0149-7634/$ - see front matter q 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.09.001
M.E. Bouton, E.W. Moody / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28 (2004) 663–674
that separate learning and performance, and thus compli-
presenting CS without the US on a number of trials
cate simple inferences about psychological processes
eliminates (‘extinguishes’) that responding. The study of
(learning, memory, emotion) from behavior.
extinction is interesting in its own right, because something
The distinction between learning and performance was
like it must be available to animals in order to allow them to
an important part of the thinking of early learning theorists.
update and modify their behavior in a changing environ-
For instance, Edward Tolman, one of the most important
ment. In addition, it has been explicitly linked to various
theorists of the 20th century, made a convincing case for it.
therapies designed to eliminate unwanted cognitions,
For example, in the well-known latent learning experiment,
emotions, and behaviors that themselves depend on learning
Tolman and Honzik allowed rats to explore a 14-unit
It is tempting to suppose that the loss of responding in
T-maze on a series of trials. One group received reward each
extinction merely reflects the destruction of the original
time it reached the end of the maze, whereas another group
learning that led to it. But we know this is not the case; the
did not. Not surprisingly, the rewarded group moved
original association is retained, perhaps fully intact. The
through the maze more efficiently, making fewer errors
extinction phenomenon is therefore another place where it is
(entering fewer dead ends) over the first 11 trials. But when
important to understand the distinction between learning
the non-rewarded group was then rewarded, they began to
(what the animal knows) and performance (what the animal
move efficiently through the maze beginning on the next
trial. According to Tolman and Honzik, the nonrewarded
There are several ways to show that the original
group had been learning about the maze the whole time,
association is intact after extinction. First, there is
even though that learning or knowledge had not been
‘reinstatement.’ In reinstatement, if the US is now presented
evident in their behavior. The function of reward was not to
a few times after extinction is complete, responding will
stamp behavior in, but instead to motivate the animal to
return to the CS when the CS is presented again One of
perform. A motivational function of reward was widely
the main reasons reinstatement occurs is that the animal
accepted in subsequent theory . Learning is not the
associates the US with the context (background stimuli
same as performance. Motivation is required for the
typically defined as emanating from the box in which the
experiment is conducted) when the US is presented after
Modern thinking has followed another of Tolman’s
extinction. When the animal is subsequently tested with the
ideas. He argued, at a time when it was not fashionable to do
CS in the context, the contextual conditioning triggers
so, that learning was not the simple attachment of a
responding to the CS. One of the most important types of
behavioral response to an environmental stimulus (so-called
evidence supporting this view is that the US must be
S-R learning). Instead, the animal represented its experience
presented in the context in which the CS will be tested. If the
in some sort of cognitive way. For instance, Tolman
animal is presented with the US in an irrelevant context, it
claimed that rats learned cognitive maps of the environment.
does not produce reinstatement Although the
Although the idea that animals learn a literal map is
contextual conditioning that causes reinstatement is not
debatable, the cognitive view of what is learned in classical
always evident in behavior directly elicited by the context,
conditioning has become dominant This view assumes
the strength of reinstatement correlates with the strength of
that there is a distinction between what is learned and what
contextual conditioning when it is measured with sensitive
is manifest in behavior. For example, theorists now suppose
context-preference tests . Reinstatement indicates
that learning involves some sort of encoding of information,
that extinction is not the same as unlearning. Responding to
storage of that information in memory, and then the retrieval
the extinguished CS can return depending on what the
of it. Learning processes thus involve the encoding and
storage of information. Performance, on the other hand,
A second phenomenon indicating that extinction is not
depends at least in part on successful retrieval. The current
unlearning, and that the CS has become dependent on the
article is mainly interested in considering how memory and
context, is the ‘renewal effect’ In renewal, an
memory retrieval processes operate in classical condition-
animal might receive conditioning trials with the CS in one
ing. An appreciation of these processes is essential for a
context (Context A) and then extinction trials in another
complete understanding of this deceptively simple learning
(Context B). When the animal is then returned to the
process. They are methodologically important because they
original context and presented with the CS, responding to
introduce a layer of complexity when inferring an animal’s
the CS recovers (is ‘renewed’). Although most studies of
learning or knowledge from behavior or performance.
renewal have used the ABA design (in which conditioning,extinction, and testing are conducted in Contexts A, B, andA, respectively), other work indicates that renewal can also
occur if the contexts are ABC or even AAB Suchresults suggest that extinction is especially dependent on the
Consider extinction, a learning phenomenon that has
context. It would be a mistake to think, based on the
been investigated in our laboratory for many years. Just as a
animal’s lack of responding in the extinction context, that
CS–US pairing comes to evoke responding, subsequently
M.E. Bouton, E.W. Moody / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28 (2004) 663–674
One of the interesting things about research on the
words. (The word ‘fire’ can mean different things, and evoke
renewal effect is that it suggests that extinction depends
different behaviors, depending on whether it is shouted in
more on the context than conditioning does. Although it
the movie theater or the shooting gallery.) Interestingly,
seems relatively easy to find that extinction performance is
occasion setters are not generally assumed to elicit
lost after a context switch, conditioning performance is
performance by themselves, as ordinary CSs might;
rarely lost when the CS is switched to a familiar context,
instead, they mainly operate by modulating performance
which would minimize possible external inhibition
effects created by novel contexts used in some experiments
The occasion setting mechanism is similar to another
. A change of context after conditioning does not
possible role of context, namely, to enable retrieval of the
attenuate conditioned fear to a CS as measured by
CS’s association with the US. As mentioned above, when
the animal is outside the extinction context, it might fail to
appetitive conditioning or taste aversion condition-
retrieve extinction, which can be taken as a CS–no US
ing The fact that extinction is more context-
association Consistent with this view, retrieval cues
dependent than conditioning is consistent with the idea that
that remind the animal of extinction can abolish the renewal
the animal codes extinction as a kind of conditional
exception to the rule—one that depends on the current
this view, the context and memory retrieval processes are
context When the context is changed, extinction
necessary in the translation of knowledge into performance
performance disappears and conditioning performance
A growing literature on the brain processes involved in
A related recovery-after-extinction phenomenon is
extinction is broadly consistent with the behavioral research
‘spontaneous recovery’. In this phenomenon, if the
just summarized (see for one review). For instance,
experimenter merely allows time to pass after extinction,
extinction appears to be linked to new brain plasticity
the extinguished response can recover . Just as
extinction is more sensitive to context than conditioning,
voltage-gated calcium channels in the shorter term .
so it is more sensitive to the effects of the passage of time.
One implication is that facilitation of these synaptic
In fact, we have argued that the passage of time affects
processes should help facilitate extinction, which has been
extinction precisely because it is a kind of context Just
shown with administration of an NMDA partial agonist,
as the renewal effect indicates that extinction is sensitive to
D-cycloserine It is not known whether facilitated
the physical context, spontaneous recovery suggests that it
extinction results from deeper extinction learning that is less
is sensitive to the context provided by time.
context-dependent or merely learning that is easier to
Research thus clearly indicates that the current perform-
retrieve in the right context. At the systems level, fear
ance elicited by a CS can underestimate what the animal
extinction may be linked in part to activity in the infralimbic
actually ‘knows’ about the CS. The major factor that
region of the medial prefrontal cortex or to
influences performance after extinction—besides the latent
possible GABAergic interneurons in the lateral amygdala
CS–US association—appears to be the current context. How
A role for hippocampus, an area long thought to be
does the context operate? Conditioning theorists have had
involved in context learning is also suggested by data
much to say about this. First, several influential conditioning
indicating that the ABC renewal effect is suppressed by
models assume that the context enters into direct associ-
inactivation of the hippocampus although the ABA
ations with the US, just as a CS might . These
renewal effect is not affected by lesions . The pattern
context-US associations would be expected to summate
may be consistent with the idea that negative occasion
with the CS–US association to generate performance.
setting by Context B, which is presumably a major source of
Unfortunately, an emphasis on this idea cannot explain
the ABC effect, is especially dependent on the hippocampus
details of the results just reviewed . For example, it is
. Reinstatement, which depends on direct associations
not consistent with the fact that an extinguished CS is
between the context and the US, also depends on an intact
especially sensitive to context-US associations and
hippocampal system in fear conditioning , though
measurable context-US associations do not seem to be
not in appetitive conditioning The various brain
processes are likely to contribute in different ways to the
A second possibility is that the contexts have the
acquisition of new learning in extinction and its suppression
properties of ‘occasion setters’ (see for
reviews). On this view, the context is not merely associatedwith the CS (or the absence of the CS), but instead selects oractivates the CS’s own current association with the US
2. Long-term memory processes in animal conditioning
Thus, the extinction context activates something like the
animal’s CS–no US association It is as if the contextdetermines the current meaning of the ambiguous CS, much
It is interesting to note that the extensive literature on
as verbal contexts disambiguate the meaning of ambiguous
long-term memory in animals also reinforces the view that it
M.E. Bouton, E.W. Moody / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28 (2004) 663–674
is important to distinguish between learning and perform-
It is worth observing that the weakness of the context
ance. The word ‘forgetting’ describes a behavioral
switch effect in humans has a parallel in the animal research.
phenomenon in which performance assessed at Time 2 is
As we noted above, a change of context after simple
shown to be inferior to that shown at Time 1, typically the
excitatory conditioning usually produces little effect on
time when the task is originally learned. Although it is
responding to the CS . This sort of result can perhaps
tempting to assume that the behavioral phenomenon is
be seen as a case of outshining: The animal is responding to
explained by the decay or erosion of the original memory
a highly salient cue for the US (the CS) that overcomes the
trace, we know that this often is not true. For example, it is
absence of contextual support in the changed context.
often possible to reverse forgetting (and apparently recover
Conditioning theories essentially argue that the CS is a more
a forgotten memory) by exposing the animal to a retrieval
informative cue than the context Other experiments in
cue This sort of result has been taken to mean that
which context switches have been said to impair memory
forgetting often results from an inability to access or retrieve
retrieval often have not included an overt CS; for example,
the target trace (see for some of the relevant
investigators may test passive avoidance , in which
the animal refrains from entering a compartment where it
Research on remembering and forgetting is often
had previously been shocked, and thus responds directly to
consistent with Tulving’s Encoding Specificity Principle,
contextual (apparatus) cues. With few cues that can
which emphasizes the importance of the similarity between
potentially outshine them, it may not be surprising to see
background contextual cues present during learning and
an effect of switching the apparatus. Interestingly, in many
testing . The general idea, which has been implicitly
cases an extant context switch effect can still be attenuated if
accepted in the extinction research presented above, is that
the animal is given an extra retrieval cue —a
retrieval depends on a match between the conditions present
potential parallel to mental reinstatement in humans. The
during learning and the conditions present during testing. In
parallel between the animal and human literatures is even
the original research with human participants, the specificity
stronger than this. The fact that the context is especially
was defined by subtle semantic shadings of target, to-be-
important after extinction is consistent with other parts of
remembered words. Tulving and Thomson showed that
the human memory literature, which indicate that context
the memory for words on a list was affected by weakly-
switch effects are similarly easier to detect in interference
associated words that were present at input, which
designs in which the participant learns a conflicting word
apparently influenced the semantic meaning encoded for
list in a second phase . Apparently analogous to
the target. Their experiments dramatically showed that these
extinction, the second word list produces context-specific
weakly-associated input words were better able to retrieve
interference with memory for the first.
the target words than were more strongly-associated words
One important implication of this work, of course, is that
that were not present at input. Even though the presence of
forgetting is not necessarily due to the erasure of the learned
the strongly-associated words at the test almost certainly
information. Instead, forgetting may be the result of a
generated the target word through free association, the
retrieval failure caused by a change of context .
target words were not recognized. Although the initial work
Forgetting might also be caused by interference emanating
manipulated input cues that could profoundly affect the
from conflicting information learned at an earlier or later
meaning of target words, subsequent work has shown that
point. Interference has a long and distinguished history
memory depends on a variety of contextual stimuli,
in human learning theory When first-learned
information interferes with memory for second-learned
Consistent with this idea, memory for verbal material in
information, we have ‘proactive interference.’ When
humans is often inferior when the room is switched
second-learned information interferes with first-learned
between learning and testing However, it is often
information, we have ‘retroactive interference’. In the
disappointingly difficult to detect this effect In a
domain of animal learning, retroactive interference is
recent meta-analysis, Smith and Vela found overall
represented by several possible paradigms, including
evidence of such context-dependent memory, but
extinction or counterconditioning (where Phase 2 interferes
suggested that it is sometimes difficult to find owing to a
with Phase 1). Proactive interference is perhaps represented
number of other processes that can help support recall at
by phenomena like latent inhibition, in which initial
the time of the test. For example, ‘outshining’ refers to the
exposure to the CS without the US can interfere with
idea that if other cues besides the context can support
conditioning when the CS and US are subsequently paired.
memory retrieval, and if they are present in both the
Interestingly, it has been common in the animal learning
training and testing context, then they will diminish any
tradition to assume that proactive and retroactive inter-
context switch effect. Contexts that are not particularly
ference effects occur because of a failure at the level of
salient also would not support a context switch effect. And
learning. As we have already seen, extinction is sometimes
humans can also defeat a context switch effect by thinking
thought to result from a destruction of the original learning.
about (mentally reinstating) the original context when they
Similarly, latent inhibition is often attributed to a failure
to learn during the second (conditioning) phase owing, for
M.E. Bouton, E.W. Moody / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28 (2004) 663–674
example, to the habituation of attention to the CS
, and deprivation state Mood effects have been
Nonetheless, extinction and latent inhibition may both
reported in humans, but, perhaps as we have seen with
follow from performance processes Consistent with
exteroceptive context effects they can be influenced by
this idea, these and other proactive and retroactive
a number of other factors that make their effects small and
interference phenomena are dependent on context (e.g.
difficult to demonstrate . In animals, there is evidence
renewal effects have been obtained in both domains) and
that stress hormones that could be correlates of emotion can
time (e.g. spontaneous recovery effects have been reported
play the role of context in a renewal design. Specifically,
in both domains) (see for a review). For example, in
Ahlers and Richardson showed that administration of
latent inhibition, if Phase 2 conditioning is assessed after a
ACTH after passive avoidance extinction renewed avoid-
delay in the latent inhibition paradigm, the conditioned
ance behavior. The idea that ACTH provided a context is
response becomes stronger, as if conditioning had been
consistent with the fact that dexamethasone (which
learned but performance was obstructed by a memory of
suppresses ACTH production) delivered during condition-
latent inhibition, which was forgotten over the delay
ing nullified this effect; ACTH needs to be part of the
. In addition, if preexposure and conditioning are
conditioning context to recover behavior after extinction.
conducted in different contexts, a return to the preexposure
We have argued that the passage of time itself provides a
context after conditioning is complete can renew latent
gradually changing context discussed above,
inhibition performance . Once again, it would be a
spontaneous recovery can be understood this way .
mistake to assume that the performance evident at a
Certain forgetting phenomena have been suggested as
particular point in time is a simple product of the CS’s
posing a challenge for this type of view , although the
challenge has been addressed and arguably resolved .
Other research supports the idea that memory also
Ultimately, it is almost universally held that ‘context’ can be
depends on internal contexts provided by drugs . In
provided by many kinds of cues and all should
‘state-dependent retention’, memory is inferior when there
is a mismatch between interoceptive drug state at the time of
Our understanding of what kinds of events reactivate
learning and testing. For example, when fear extinction is
memories is not complete. As implied above, one of the
conducted while the rat is under the influence of a
main ideas is that presenting aspects of the original training
benzodiazepine (chlordiazepoxide or diazepam), fear is
situation is best for reactivating a target memory. Often, an
renewed when the animal is subsequently tested in the sober
effective cue can be thought of as part of the original
state Fear extinction can thus be dependent on the drug
context. Thus, the extinction cue in the experiments
context. State-dependent retention has important methodo-
mentioned earlier worked to retrieve extinction
logical implications for studies of the biological basis of
because it was coded as part of the extinction context;
learning and memory, where it is common to give a drug
presentation of even a part of a context can presumably
during or soon after training and then test memory in the
trigger completion of the entire pattern of the context that it
absence of the drug. Although the drug may often be of
has been associated with As we will discuss in a later
interest because it potentially disrupts a biological con-
section, a CS is often thought to evoke behavior because it
solidation process, if memory is being tested without a drug
retrieves or activates a representation of the US. This
that was otherwise present during learning and storage, then
implies that the laws of associative learning as developed in
retrieval failure is also a candidate explanation for poor
research on classical conditioning may be relevant to
performance during testing. For example, chemicals that are
understanding how retrieval cues are learned and estab-
supposed to disable learning and consolidation processes,
lished However, the effectiveness of different cues at
such as the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin and
the time of testing might depend further on poorly-
several NMDA antagonists (e.g. MK-801, ketamine,
understood details about the timing of their presentation
phencyclidine, and CGS 19155) can also generate state-
with respect to the memory test , the duration of their
dependent retention effects under some conditions (aniso-
presentation , and what other cues are presented along
with them In addition, the effectiveness of
means that performance tests in the absence of the chemical
different cues may wax and wane as the retention interval
might overestimate its impact on learning as opposed to
increases The area would benefit from more
retrieval. Such a possibility highlights the importance of
systematic, theory-driven research on the specific factors
using control conditions in which learning is tested in the
presence of the same drugged state; if a drug affects learning
Another complication is that activation of a forgotten
or consolidation when given at the time of conditioning, it
memory is not a neutral event, but itself enables further
should later affect performance in either the drugged or the
learning. For instance, when forgotten memories are
reactivated in a new context, the new context becomes
Other research suggests that context effects can be
effective at retrieving the memory, as if it has been added
created by a variety of contextual cues, including hormones
to the original training memory . (The word
(particularly in non-physiological doses) , time of day
‘reactivation’ is sometimes meant to imply susceptibility
M.E. Bouton, E.W. Moody / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28 (2004) 663–674
to further modification.) In addition, events that disrupt
themselves, until all the baits have been removed .
memory when they are administered soon after learning can
These literatures provide insights into the nature of short-
also disrupt memory when they are administered soon after
term memory processes in animals. For example, in both
reactivation. For example, Misanin et al. found that
delayed match-to-sample and the radial maze, animals seem
exposure to electroconvulsive shock soon after a reactiva-
able to code short-term information either retrospectively
tion treatment made the memory difficult to retrieve. Similar
(they remember the previous sample or previous arms that
evidence has been produced more recently by Nader et al.
have been visited) or prospectively (they remember the
who found that introduction of anisomycin likewise
correct upcoming comparison stimulus or arms that are
abolished a reactivated memory (see also for a related
yet to be visited) The implications for studies
effect of MK-801). The findings suggest that reactivation
of classical conditioning have mainly been discussed in
can make a memory susceptible to disruption again, and that
studies of occasion setting, where a feature CS informs the
similar neurobiological processes are engaged after initial
subject of whether or not a subsequent target CS will be
learning and reactivation, although they say little about the
mechanisms of retrieval itself One danger is to assume
There is also evidence that short-term memory processes
that the amnestic effect of any agent (e.g. ECS, anisomycin,
are important even in simple cases of classical conditioning.
or MK-801) is necessarily to abolish reconsolidation. For
Consistent with thinking about human memory processing,
example, although the delivery of ECS soon after learning
current theory assumes that conditioning requires some
was originally thought to abolish consolidation
processing of the CS and US together in short-term memory
subsequent work suggested that it merely made the memory
after individual conditioning trials for the association to be
more difficult to retrieve Thus, the trace had been
stored in long-term memory. In a classic experiment,
encoded after all. We have already seen that anisomycin and
Wagner et al. showed that learning of a particular
MK-801 might affect retrievability of a memory rather than
CS–US relation was damaged in rabbits if a surprising
merely disrupting consolidation . The point is that if
episode was presented within 300 s after each learning trial.
reactivation treatments initiate new learning, an amnestic
The role of short-term memory in conditioning has
agent may either interfere with reconsolidation or make
figured importantly in the influential theories of Wagner
the reconsolidated memory more difficult to retrieve.
The original formulation of his model, which
The distinction between learning and performance is
paralleled models of human information processing
explicitly proposed that storage of the CS–US
It is also interesting to note that reactivation treatments
association in long-term memory depended on the CS and
that involve exposure to a CS operationally resemble
US being processed (‘rehearsed’) in short-term memory
extinction trials. Typically, however, CS exposures that
after each conditioning trial. Importantly, surprising CSs
reactivate and engage reconsolidation do not create extinc-
and USs—specifically, those that are not already ‘primed,’
tion (the CS improves performance, rather than weakens it,
or currently represented in short-term memory, when they
compared to control groups that receive no CS). It is possible
are presented in a trial—were assumed to command more
that extinction learning, which involves the learning of new
rehearsal than those that are not surprising. Priming can
conflicting information, simply depends on more exposure to
occur either through recent presentation of the event itself or
the CS. The relationship between these processes is
through recent presentation of a retrieval cue associated
interesting and important, but poorly understood at present.
with the event (which would retrieve the item from long-term memory and put it into short-term memory). Furthermore, consistent with an important characteristic of
3. Short-term memory processes in conditioning
working memory in humans, the capacity of short-termmemory was assumed to be limited. These ideas and
Short-term memory processes have also been important
assumptions allowed the model to explain am impressive
in studies of animal learning. For example, there is a
amount of new data (see for review).
substantial amount of interest in variations of the delayed
In a more recent version of the model, Wagner
matching to sample procedure, in which animals are given a
put the ideas in a connectionist framework. In this
sample stimulus at one point in time and then given that
model, known as the ‘sometimes opponent process’ model,
stimulus and another a few seconds later, when they are
or ‘SOP,’ CSs and USs are assumed to have corresponding
required to respond to the stimulus that matches the sample
‘nodes’ in memory. The presentation of a CS or US is
in order to acquire reinforcement With typical
assumed to activate the node. Initially, the node is activated
methods, the pigeon’s choice returns to chance when the
to a highly active state (‘A1’), but this quickly decays to a
interval between the presentation of the sample and then the
less active state (‘A2’), where it stays a bit longer before
choice stimuli is in the order of many seconds Short
returning to the inactive (I) state. (An activated node is
term memory for recent choices similarly influences
essentially one that is represented in short-term memory.)
behavior on the radial maze, where rats choose among
The animal learns about the CS only when it has been put
a number of baited arms, almost without repeating
into its maximally active state (A1). If the US node is
M.E. Bouton, E.W. Moody / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28 (2004) 663–674
simultaneously activated to the same A1 state, then the
presented on trials that are more spaced in time, there may
animal will learn a connection or association between the
be a significant increase in conditioned responding on the
two. As a consequence of conditioning, the CS is able to
spaced trials On the other hand, the increase in
activate the US node into the A2 (not the A1) state; this
responding does not reach the level in animals that have
activation ultimately produces a conditioned response.
received spaced trials throughout training . This
Notice that if time were to elapse between presentation
result might suggest that massed trials also have an enduring
of the CS and US, the CS could decay from A1 to the less-
effect on learning, although it is also possible that the
active A2 state and therefore be unavailable for learning.
animals trained with massed trials and tested with spaced
The theory actually supposes that nodes are composed of
ones might not generalize perfectly between trials presented
many elements that move from A1 to A2 and then
on the two schedules. The evidence suggests that recent
Inactivity in a probabilistic fashion. Given these dynamics,
presentations of the conditioning events can generate a
the theory explains the results of ‘trace conditioning’
short-term suppressive effect on performance and might
experiments, in which one observes increasingly poor
conditioning as the temporal gap between CS offset and US
Of course, priming in short-term memory can only
onset increases 1 The US node similarly decays from
account for the effects of trial massing within the range of
A1 to A2 to Inactivity after each US presentation, although
short intertrial intervals. In our appetitive conditioning
here the consequences are somewhat different. If the US is
experiments in which rats are given pairings of 10- or 30-s
in A2 at the time the CS is in A1, an inhibitory CS–US
CSs with food pellet USs, priming effects are clearly evident
association is learned. This is one account of inhibitory
when the trials are separated by 60 s, but not when they are
conditioning that can occur in ‘backward conditioning,’
separated by 240 s It is worth noting that
when the CS is presented relatively soon after the offset of
additional spacing of trials beyond 240 s can have additional
positive effects on conditioning This fact implies that
The dynamics of short-term memory in SOP can go some
additional mechanisms do contribute to trial-spacing effects
distance in explaining a number of other interesting effects
(see for review). Our own research suggests that long
in conditioning. For example, recent work in our own
intertrial intervals may create better conditioning because
laboratory has investigated the well-known trial-spacing
they allow extinction of contextual cues that receive
effect, in which trials that are spaced relatively widely in
conditioning during CS–US pairings These otherwise
time produce better conditioning than trials that are massed
compete with (or block conditioning of the CS.
in time Although a number of explanations of
Quantitative conditioning models suppose exactly this
this effect have been proposed recent research
process . SOP gives the process psychological
suggests that massing trials closely together in time may
flesh by arguing that conditioned contextual cues wouldblock CS conditioning because they activate the US node
make conditioning inferior because the presentation of CS
to A2, and thus reduce the surprisingness of the US when it
and US on one trial primes their representations into short-
term memory (the A2 state) and makes them less surprising
intertrial intervals weakens the context’s ability to put the
on the next trial The results were not consistent
To summarize the trial-distribution findings, very short
Interestingly, although it is once again tempting to
intertrial intervals can hurt conditioning because they prime
assume that priming reduces learning, massed training may
the CS and US in short-term memory, making them less
have its clearest effect on performance. For example,
likely to command performance and/or learning on the next
presentation of the CS a few seconds before it is presented
trial. At longer intertrial intervals, the context becomes less
again can reduce responding on the second CS presentation
likely to retrieve the representation of the US (and perhaps
after conditioning has already occurred . When
the CS) and therefore allows better conditioning. Several
conditioning occurs in a within-subject procedure that
psychological mechanisms thus play a role in trial-spacing
intermixes short and long intertrial intervals, there may be
effects. But a productive way to conceptualize them is to
less responding in the short intertrial intervals a
emphasize the influence priming effects in short-term
learning deficit caused by the short intervals should be
manifest in behavior at all intervals. And when a CS that hasreceived conditioning in a massed-trial procedure is
1 SOP also allows other mechanisms to contribute to the trace
conditioning deficit. If the US occurs alone at the end of a very long gap
Our discussion to this point has accepted the idea that
(‘trace interval’), it could be associated with contextual cues that are present
once conditioning has occurred, the CS functions as a
at the time Contextual conditioning would reduce any possible
retrieval cue that activates a US node or representation. The
conditioning of the CS, because it would allow the context to activate
idea has received direct empirical support. For example,
(prime) the US node to the A2 state, reducing its surprisingness andtherefore causing blocking
Rescorla conditioned fear in rats by pairing a light CS
M.E. Bouton, E.W. Moody / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28 (2004) 663–674
with a loud noise (created by a klaxon). In a subsequent
Also importantly, how the animal currently values the
phase, he presented the klaxon repeatedly by itself to
representation of the outcome in instrumental learning
habituate fear to it. During a third phase, he presented the CS
depends upon a subtle process known as ‘incentive
alone. At this point, the CS elicited less fear than it did in a
control group that did not receive habituation to the noise.
trained rats to lever-press for food pellets while they were
Rescorla argued that habituation had modified the rat’s
satiated and then tested lever pressing in extinction. At
memory representation of the noise; when the CS was then
this time, the rats were either hungry or not. Hunger had
presented again, it activated a less frightening US represen-
no effect on the level of responding. But if the rats had
tation. Other experiments revealed a corresponding inflation
been given a separate opportunity to eat the pellets while
effect rats that received fear conditioning with a
they were hungry, hunger strongly increased responding in
weak-intensity shock US showed augmented fear of the CS
the extinction test. The animal needed to learn about the
if they were then exposed to more intense shocks. The
effect of food on hunger. Hunger thus motivates behavior
argument is thus that the response evoked by a CS depends
in a subtle way; the organism will perform an action while
in part on the status of the animal’s US representation. A CS
hungry if the behavior has been associated with a
may generate behavior at least in part because it serves as a
particular reinforcer, and if the reinforcer has been
retrieval cue for that representation.
associated with something like amelioration of the hunger
These findings have been extended in a number of ways.
For instance, Holland has presented striking evidence
The evidence thus suggests that animals form relatively
that the rat forms a rich representation of the US, and that
rich representations as a result of conditioning. One effect of
responding to the CS depends on the status of that
a CS is to retrieve a representation of the US. Consistent
representation. In the basic experiment, rats receive pairings
with this, conditioned responding (or performance) is a
of different tone CSs with differently-flavored sucrose
product of the animal’s knowledge of a CS–US or response-
solutions. When one of these is then separately paired
reinforcer association and how the animal currently values
with illness (which creates a conditioned aversion to the
the US. The assignment of value, especially in the operant
flavor), the rat exhibits less appetitive responding to the
situation, involves motivational as well as memory
paired tone during final tests. Remarkably, as if the tone
processes, which brings us full circle to the ideas of Tolman
evoked an almost palpable image of the flavor it was
that we mentioned at the start of this article.
associated with, rats reacted to novel combinations of tonesin a way that paralleled what they had separately learnedabout the corresponding combinations of flavors. In
addition, the effect of taste-aversion revaluation of the USappears to be less noticeable when conditioning involves a
This short review has only scratched the surface of the
more extended number of CS–US pairings, as if conditioned
complexity of classical conditioning. In addition to
responding becomes more ‘automatic,’ rather than rep-
describing some of the many roles for memory in
resentation-mediated, with extended exposure to the
conditioning, we have repeatedly noted that any study of
conditioning must always distinguish learning from per-
Related findings have been reported in operant con-
formance. We have seen that extinction does not depend on
ditioning, where animals learn to associate a behavior (such
unlearning; erased performance at the end of extinction does
as lever pressing) with a reinforcer instead of a CS with a
not reflect erased knowledge. Instead, performance after
US. In this case, there is a similar literature indicating that
extinction is a product of the animal’s knowledge of two
the animal associates one event (the response) with a
conflicting associations, and how the context selects
representation of the outcome. In a simple experiment, if a
between them. Similarly, forgetting in studies of long-
rat is trained to lever-press for a food pellet, separate
term memory does not necessarily result from erasure or
pairings of the pellet and illness will condition an aversion
decay, but instead often results from reduced access, either
to the pellet—and will also cause the rat to suppress its
due to retrieval failure or interference. Once again, the
performance of the operant when it is tested in extinction
context plays a role. Short-term memory effects are also
As in classical conditioning, the current amount
evident in classical conditioning; in this case, recent
of responding depends on the strength of a hypothetical
presentations of events can temporarily suppress perform-
association and the extent to which the animal currently
ance and learning. Finally, the idea that a CS functions as a
values the outcome. Interestingly, extended training in
retrieval cue provides another link between conditioning
which the rat has many response-outcome pairings can
and memory, as well as a further separation between what
decrease the sensitivity of the response to the devaluation
Although we have focused primarily on memory
behavior has become ‘automatized,’ i.e. more habit-like and
processes, a number of other factors also matter in the
less dependent on the current status of the representation of
translation of learning into performance. We have barely
mentioned motivational factors (see for one recent
M.E. Bouton, E.W. Moody / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28 (2004) 663–674
review). In addition, the qualitative nature of the CS can
[12] Bouton ME, Bolles RC. Role of conditioned contextual stimuli in
influence the type of conditioned response one observes
reinstatement of extinguished fear. J Exp Psychol Anim BehavProcess 1979;5:368–78.
So can the duration of the CS: CSs that end in the
[13] Bouton ME, King DA. Contextual control of the extinction of
US after a relatively brief interval can evoke conditioned
conditioned fear: tests for the associative value of the context. J Exp
responses that are qualitatively different from those evoked
Psychol Anim Behav Process 1983;9:248–65.
by CSs that end in the US after a longer interval
[14] Frohardt RJ, Guarraci FA, Bouton ME. The effects of neurotoxic
In any of these cases, it would be a mistake to make
hippocampal lesions on two effects of context after fear extinction. Behav Neurosci 2000;114:227–40.
inferences about learning from measurements of a single
[15] Wilson A, Brooks DC, Bouton ME. The role of the rat hippocampal
response, a point that has been made in other animal
system in several effects of context in extinction. Behav Neurosci
learning paradigms as well The behavioral
product of classical conditioning is not a single, monolithic
[16] Bouton ME, Bolles RC. Contextual control of the extinction of
conditioned response. Conditioning instead engages whole
conditioned fear. Learn Motiv 1979;10:445–66.
[17] Bouton ME, Brooks DC. Time and context effects on performance in
behavior systems, or sets or constellations of behaviors that
a Pavlovian discrimination reversal. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav
are evolutionarily adapted to help optimize the organism’s
interaction with the US Although the topic of
[18] Bouton ME, Ricker ST. Renewal of extinguished responding in a
behavior systems is beyond the scope of the present article,
second context. Anim Learn Behav 1994;22:317–24.
it further reminds us that classical conditioning involves a
[19] Penick K, Solomon PR. Hippocampus, context and conditioning.
sophisticated and surprisingly varied set of behavioral
[20] Devenport L. Sampling behavior and contextual change. Learn
processes that will always come into play whenever
conditioning is used as a tool to study the brain.
[21] Gisquet-Verrier P, Alexinsky T. Does contextual change determine
long-term forgetting? Anim Learn Behav 1986;14:349–58.
[22] Hall G, Honey RC. Contextual effects in conditioning, latent
inhibition, and habituation: associative and retrieval functions of
contextual cues. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 1989;15:232–41.
[23] Kaye H, Mackintosh NJ. A change of context can enhance
Preparation of this manuscript was supported by Grant
performance of an aversive but not of an appetitive conditioned
RO1 MH64847 from the National Institute of Mental Health
response. Q J Exp Psychol B 1990;42:113–34.
to MEB. We thank Jaylyn Waddell and Ceyhun Sunsay for
[24] Harris JA, Jones ML, Bailey GK, Westbrook RF. Contextual control
their discussion and comments. Send correspondence to
over conditioned responding in an extinction paradigm. J Exp
Mark E. Bouton, Department of Psychology, University of
Psychol Anim Behav Process 2000;26:174–85.
[25] Bouton ME, Peck CA. Context effects on conditioning, extinction,
and reinstatement in an appetitive conditioning preparation. AnimLearn Behav 1989;17:188–98.
[26] Rosas JM, Bouton ME. Renewal of a conditioned taste aversion upon
return to the conditioning context after extinction in another one.
[27] Rosas JM, Bouton ME. Context change and retention interval can
[1] Tolman EC, Honzik CH. Introduction and removal of reward, and
have additive, rather than interactive, effects after taste aversion
maze performance in rats. U Cal Pub Psych 1930;4:257–75.
extinction. Psychon Bull Rev 1998;5:79–83.
[2] Hull CL. A behavior system; an introduction to behavior theory
[28] Bouton ME. Conditioning, remembering, and forgetting. J Exp
concerning the individual organism. New Haven: Yale University
Psychol Anim Behav Process 1994;20:219–31.
[29] Brooks DC, Bouton ME. A retrieval cue for extinction attenuates
[3] Spence KW. Behavior theory and conditioning. New Haven: Yale
spontaneous recovery. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 1993;19:
[4] Mowrer OH. Learning theory and behavior. New York: Wiley; 1960.
[30] Pavlov IP. Conditioned reflexes. Oxford, UK: Oxford University
[5] Rescorla RA, Solomon RL. Two-process learning theory: relation-
ships between Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning.
[31] Robbins SJ. Mechanisms underlying spontaneous recovery in
autoshaping. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 1990;16:235–49.
[6] Tolman EC. Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychol Rev 1948;55:
[32] Bouton ME. Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference
paradigms of Pavlovian learning. Psychol Bull 1993;114:80–99.
[7] Hulse SH, Fowler H, Honig WK. Cognitive processes in animal
[33] Mackintosh NJ. A theory of attention: variations in the associability
behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1978.
of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychol Rev 1975;82:276–98.
[8] Rescorla RA. Pavlovian conditioning: it’s not what you think it is.
[34] Pearce JM, Hall G. A model for Pavlovian conditioning: variations in
the effectiveness of conditioned but not unconditioned stimuli.
[9] Barlow DH. Anxiety and its disorders: the nature and treatment of
anxiety and panic, 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press; 2002.
[35] Rescorla RA, Wagner AR. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning:
[10] Rescorla RA, Heth CD. Reinstatement of fear to an extinguished
variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforce-
conditioned stimulus. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 1975;1:
ment. In: Black AH, Prokasy WK, editors. Classical conditioning II:
current research and theory. New York: Appleton–Century–Crofts;
[11] Bouton ME. Differential control by context in the inflation and
reinstatement paradigms. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 1984;
[36] Wagner AR. A model of automatic memory processing in animal
behavior. In: Spear NE, Miller RR, editors. Information processing
M.E. Bouton, E.W. Moody / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28 (2004) 663–674
in animals: memory mechanisms. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1981. p.
[57] Harris JA, Westbrook RF. Evidence that GABA transmission
mediates context-specific extinction of learned fear. Psychopharma-
[37] Wagner AR, Brandon SE. Evolution of a structured connectionist
model of Pavlovian conditioning (AESOP). In: Klein SB,
[58] Holland PC, Bouton ME. Hippocampus and context in classical
Mowrer RR, editors. Contemporary learning theories: Pavlovian
conditioning. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1999;9:195–202.
conditioning and the status of traditional learning theory. Hillsdale,
[59] Corcoran KA, Maren S. Hippocampal inactivation disrupts con-
textual retrieval of fear memory after extinction. J Neurosci 2001;21:
[38] Bouton ME. Context and retrieval in extinction and in other
examples of interference in simple associative learning. In:
[60] Frohardt RJ, Guarraci FA, Bouton ME. The effects of neurotoxic
Dachowski L, Flaherty CF, editors. Current topics in animal
hippocampal lesions on two effects of context after fear extinction.
learning: brain, emotion, and cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum;
[61] Wilson A, Brooks DC, Bouton ME. The role of the rat hippocampal
[39] Pearce JM, Bouton ME. Theories of associative learning in animals.
system in several effects of context in extinction. Behav Neurosci
[40] Bouton ME, King DA. Effect of context on performance to
[62] Holland PC, Lamoureux JA, Han JS, Gallagher M. Hippocampal
conditioned stimuli with mixed histories of reinforcement and
lesions interfere with Pavlovian negative occasion setting. Hippo-
nonreinforcement. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 1986;12:
[63] Fox GD, Holland PC. Neurotoxic hippocampal lesions fail to impair
[41] Bouton ME, Swartzentruber D. Analysis of the associative and
reinstatement of an appetitively conditioned response. Behav
occasion-setting properties of contexts participating in a Pavlovian
discrimination. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 1986;12:333–50.
[64] Spear NE. The processing of memories: forgetting and retention.
[42] Holland PC. Occasion setting in Pavlovian conditioning. In:
Bower G, editor. The psychology of learning and motivation, vol.
[65] Sara SJ. Retrieval and reconsolidation: toward a neurobiology of
28. Orlando, FL: Academic Press; 1992. p. 69–125.
remembering. Learn Mem 2000;7:73–84.
[43] Swartzentruber D. Modulatory mechanisms in Pavlovian condition-
[66] Tulving E, Thomson DM. Encoding specificity and retrieval
ing. Anim Learn Behav 1995;23:123–43.
processes in episodic memory. Psychol Rev 1973;80:352–73.
[44] Bouton ME, Nelson JB. Mechanisms of feature-positive and feature-
[67] Godden DR, Baddeley AD. Context-dependent memory in two
negative discrimination learning in an appetitive conditioning
natural environments: on land and underwater. Br J Psychol 1975;66:
paradigm. In: Schmajuk NA, Holland PC, editors. Occasion setting:
associative learning and cognition in animals. Washington, DC:
[68] Smith SM. Remembering in and out of context. J Exp Psychol Hum
American Psychological Association; 1998. p. 69–112.
[45] Brooks DC, Bouton ME. A retrieval cue for extinction attenuates
response recovery (renewal) caused by a return to the conditioning
Davies GM, Thomson DM, editors. Memory in context: context in
context. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 1994;20:366–79.
memory. New York: Wiley; 1988. p. 13–34.
[46] Brooks DC. Recent and remote extinction cues reduce spontaneous
[70] Smith SM, Vela E. Environmental context-dependent memory: a
recovery. Q J Exp Psychol 2000;53B:25–58.
review and meta-analysis. Psychon Bull Rev 2001;8:203–20.
[47] Brooks DC, Palmatier MI, Garcia EO, Johnson JL. An extinction cue
[71] Land CL, Riccio DC. Nonmonotonic changes in the context shift
reduces spontaneous recovery of a conditioned taste aversion. Anim
effect over time. Learn Motiv 1998;29:280–7.
[72] Zhou Y, Riccio DC. Manipulation of components of context: the
[48] Myers KM, Davis M. Behavioral and neural analysis of extinction.
context shift effect and forgetting of stimulus attributes. Learn Motiv
[49] Falls WA, Miserendo MJD, Davis M. Extinction of fear-potentiated
[73] Gordon WC, McCracken KM, Dess-Beech N, Mowrer RR. Mech-
startle: blockade by infusion of an NMDA antagonist into the
anisms for the cueing phenomenon: the addition of the cuing context
amygdala. J Neurosci 1992;12:853–63.
[50] Santini E, Muller RU, Quirk GJ. Consolidation of extinction learning
to the training memory. Learn Motiv 1981;12:196–211.
involves transfer from NMDA-independent to NMDA-dependent
[74] Mowrer RR, Gordon WC. Effects of cuing in an ‘irrelevant’ context.
memory. J Neurosci 2001;21:9009–17.
[51] Cain C, Blouin A, Barad MG. L-type voltage-gated calcium channels
[75] Wittrup M, Gordon WC. Alteration of training memory through
are required for extinction, but not for acquisition or expression, of
cueing. Am J Psychol 1982;95:497–507.
conditioned fear in mice. J Neurosci 2002;22:9113–21.
[76] McGeoch JA. Forgetting and the law of disuse. Psychol Rev 1932;
[52] Ledgerwood L, Richardson R, Cranney J. Effects of
extinction of conditioned freezing. Behav Neurosci 2003;117:341–9.
[77] Postman L, Underwood BJ. Critical issues in interference theory.
[53] Walker DL, Ressler KJ, Lu KT, Davis M. Facilitation of conditioned
fear extinction by systemic administration or intra-amygdala
[78] Kraemer PJ, Spear NE. The effect of nonreinforced stimulus
infusions of D-cycloserine as assessed by fear potentiated startle in
exposure on the strength of a conditioned taste aversion as a
function of retention interval: Do latent inhibition and extinction
[54] Millad MR, Quirk GJ. Neurons in medial prefrontal cortex signal
involve a shared process? Anim Learn Behav 1992;20:1–7.
memory for fear extinction. Nature 2002;420:70–4.
[79] Aguado L, Symonds M, Hall G. Interval between preexposure and
[55] Gewirtz JC, Falls WA, Davis M. Normal conditioning inhibition and
test determines the magnitude of latent inhibition: implications for
extinction of freezing and fear-potentiated startle following electro-
an interference account. Anim Learn Behav 1994;22:188–94.
lytic lesions of medial prefrontal cortex in rats. Behav Neurosci
[80] Bouton ME, Swartzentruber D. Slow reacquisition following
extinction: context, encoding, and retrieval mechanisms. J Exp
[56] Goosens KA, Maren S. Pretraining NMDA receptor blockade in the
Psychol Anim Behav Process 1989;15:43–53.
basolateral complex, but not the central nucleus, of the amygdala
[81] Maren S, Holt W. The hippocampus and contextual memory
prevents savings of the conditional fear. Behav Neurosci 2003;117:
retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning. Behav Brain Res 2000;110:
M.E. Bouton, E.W. Moody / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28 (2004) 663–674
[82] Westbrook RF, Jones ML, Bailey GK, Harris JA. Contextual control
[107] Miller RR, Springer AD. Retrieval failure induced by electrocon-
over conditioned responding in a latent inhibition paradigm. J Exp
vulsive shock: reversal with dissimilar training and recovery agents.
Psychol Anim Behav Process 2000;26:157–73.
[83] Overton DA. Contextual stimulus effects of drugs and internal states.
[108] Blough DS. Delayed matching in the pigeon. J Exp Anal Behav
In: Balsam PD, Tomie A, editors. Context and learning. Hillsdale,
[109] Roberts WA, Grant DS. Studies of short-term memory in the pigeon
[84] Bouton ME, Kenny FA, Rosengard C. State-dependent fear
using delayed matching to sample procedure. In: Medin DL,
extinction with two benzodiazepine tranquilizers. Behav Neurosci
Roberts WA, Davis RT, editors. Processes of animal memory.
Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum; 1976. p. 79–112.
[85] Radyushkin KA, Anokhin KV. Recovery of memory in chicks after
[110] Olton DS, Samuelson RJ. Remembrance of places passed: spatial
disruption during learning: the reversibility of amnesia induced by
memory in rats. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 1976;2:97–116.
protein synthesis inhibitors. Neurosci Behav Physiol 1999;29:31–6.
[111] Cook RG, Brown MF, Riley DA. Flexible memory processing by
[86] Harrod SB, Flint RW, Riccio DC. MK-801 induced retrieval, but not
rats: use of prospective and retrospective information in the radial
acquisition, deficits for passive avoidance conditioning. Pharmacol
maze. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 1985;11:453–69.
[112] Zentall TR, Urcuioli PJ, Jackson-Smith P, Steirn JN. Memory
[87] Jackson A, Koek W, Colpaert FC. NMDA antagonists make learning
strategies in pigeons. In: Dachowski L, Flaherty CF, editors. Current
and recall state-dependent. Behav Pharmacol 1992;3:415–21.
topics in animal learning: brain, emotion, and cognition. Hillsdale,
[88] Costanzo DJ, Riccio DC, Kissinger S. State-dependent retention
produced with estrus in rats. Physiol Behav 1995;57:1009–11.
[113] Swartzentruber D. Perspectives on modulation: modulator- and
[89] Holloway FA, Wansley RA. Multiple retention deficits at periodic
target-focused views. In: Schmajuk NA, Holland PC, editors.
intervals after active and passive avoidance learning. Behav Biol
Occasion setting: associative learning and cognition in animals.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1998. p.
[90] Davidson TL. The nature and function of interoceptive signals to
feed: toward integration of physiological and learning perspectives.
[114] Wagner AR, Rudy JW, Whitlow JW. Rehearsal in animal
conditioning. J Exp Psychol 1973;97:407–26.
[115] Wagner AR. Expectancies and the priming of STM. In: Hulse SH,
[91] Eich E. Searching for mood dependent memory. Psychol Sci 1995;6:
Fowler H, Hoing WK, editors. Cognitive aspects of animal behavior.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1978. p. 177–210.
[92] Ahlers ST, Richardson R. Administration of dexamethasone prior to
[116] Atkinson RC, Shiffrin RM. Human memory: a proposed system and
training blocks ACTH-induced recovery of an extinguished
its control processes. In: Spence KW, Spence JT, editors. The
avoidance response. Behav Neurosci 1985;99:760–4.
psychology of learning and motivation, vol. 2. New York: Academic
[93] Riccio DC, Richardson R, Ebner DL. Memory retrieval deficits
based upon altered contextual cues: a paradox. Psychol Bull 1984;
[117] Wagner AR. Priming in STM: an information processing mechanism
for self-generated or retrieval-generated depression in performance.
[94] Bouton ME, Nelson JB, Rosas JM. Stimulus generalization, context
In: Tighe TJ, Leaton RN, editors. Habituation: perspectives from
change, and forgetting. Psychol Bull 1999;125:171–86.
child development, animal behavior and neurophysiology. Hillsdale,
[95] Bouton ME. Context, ambiguity, and unlearning: sources of relapse
after behavioral extinction. Biol Psychiatry 2002;52:976–86.
[118] Wagner AR, Brandon SE. A componential theory of Pavlovian
[96] Spear NE, Riccio DC. Memory: phenomena and principles. Boston,
conditioning. In: Mowrer RR, Klein SB, editors. Handbook
of contemporary learning theories. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 2001.
[97] Rudy JW, O’Reilly RC. Contextual fear conditioning, conjunctive
representations, pattern completion, and the hippocampus. Behav
[119] Smith MC, Coleman SR, Gormezano I. Classical conditioning of the
rabbit’s nicatating membrane response at backward, simultaneous
[98] Gordon WC, Smith GJ, Katz DS. Dual effects of response blocking
and forward CS–US intervals. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1969;69:
following avoidance learning. Behav Res Ther 1979;17:479–87.
[99] Miller JS, Jagielo JA, Spear NE. Differential effectiveness of various
[120] Maier SF, Rapaport P, Wheatly KL. Conditioned inhibition and the
prior-cuing treatments in the reactivation and maintenance of
UCS–CS interval. Anim Learn Behav 1976;4:217–20.
memory. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 1991;17:249–58.
[121] McNish KA, Betts SL, Brandon SE, Wagner AR. Divergence of
[100] Arnold HM, Spear NE. Order and duration of stimuli are important
conditioned eyeblink and conditioned fear in backward Pavlovian
determinants of reactivation. Anim Learn Behav 1993;(2):391–8.
training. Anim Learn Behav 1997;25:43–52.
[101] Gisquet-Verrier P. Coherence of retrieval cues, rather than
[122] Barela PB. Theoretical mechanisms underlying the trial-spacing
additivity, determines prior cuing effectiveness in the rat. Anim
effect in Pavlovian fear conditioning. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav
[102] Gisquet-Verrier P, Dekeyne A, Alexinsky T. Differential effects of
[123] Holland PC. Trial and intertrial durations in appetitive conditioning
several retrieval cues over time: evidence for time dependent
in rats. Anim Learn Behav 2000;28:121–35.
reorganization of memory. Anim Learn Behav 1989;17:394–408.
[124] Terrace HS, Gibbon J, Farrell L, Baldock MD. Temporal factors
[103] Misanin JR, Miller RR, Lewis DJ. Retrograde amnesia produced be
influencing the acquisition and maintenance of an autoshaped
electroconvulsive shock after reactivation of a consolidated memory
keypeck. Anim Learn Behav 1975;3:53–62.
[125] Gallistel CR, Gibbon J. Time, rate, and conditioning. Psychol Rev
[104] Nader K, Schafe GE, Le Doux JE. Fear memories require protein
synthesis in the amygdala for reconsolidation after retrieval. Nature
[126] Bouton ME, Sunsay C. Importance of trials versus accumulating
time across trials in partially reinforced appetitive conditioning.
[105] Przybyslawski J, Sara SJ. Reconsolidation of memory after its
J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 2003;29:62–77.
reactivation. Behav Brain Res 1997;84:241–6.
[127] Sunsay C, Stetson L, Bouton ME. Memory priming and trial
[106] Duncan CP. The retroactive effect of electroshock on learning.
spacing effects in appetitive Pavlovian learning. Learn Behav 2004;
J Comp Physiol Psychol 1949;42:32–44.
M.E. Bouton, E.W. Moody / Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 28 (2004) 663–674
[128] Pfautz PL, Wagner AR. Transient variations in responding to
[142] Balleine BW. Instrumental performance following a shift in primary
Pavlovian conditioned stimuli have implications for the mechanisms
motivation depends on incentive learning. J Exp Psychol Anim
of ‘priming’. Anim Learn Behav 1976;4:107–12.
[129] Lattal KM. Trial and intertrial durations in Pavlovian conditioning:
[143] Dickinson A, Balleine B. The role of learning in the operation of
issues of learning and performance. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav
motivational systems. In: Pashle H, Gallistel R, editors. Steven’s
handbook of experimental psychology. Learning, motivation, and
[130] Barnet RC, Grahame NJ, Miller RR. Trial spacing effects in
emotion, vol. 3. New York: Wiley; 2002. p. 497–533.
Pavlovian conditioning: a role for local context. Anim Learn Behav
[144] Holland PC. Conditioned stimulus as a determinant of the form of the
Pavlovian conditioned response. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process
[131] Sunsay C, Bouton ME. A theoretical investigation of the trial spacing
effect in appetitive Pavlovian conditioning; In Preparation.
[145] Timberlake W, Grant DL. Auto-shaping in rats to the presentation of
[132] Kamin LJ. Predictability, surprise, attention, and conditioning. In:
another rat predicting food. Science 1975;190:690–2.
Campbell BA, Church RM, editors. Punishment and aversive
[146] Akins CK. Effects of species-specific cues and the CS–US interval
behavior. New York: Appleton–Century–Crofts; 1969. p. 279–96.
on the topography of the sexually conditioned response. Learn Motiv2000;31:211–35.
[133] Rescorla RA. Effect of US habituation following conditioning.
[147] Akins CK, Domjan M, Gutie´rrez G. Topography of sexually
J Comp Physiol Psychol 1973;82:137–43.
conditioned behavior in male Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica)
[134] Rescorla RA. Effect of inflation of the unconditioned stimulus value
depends on the CS–US interval. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process
following conditioning. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1974;86:101–6.
[135] Holland PC. Event representation in Pavlovian conditioning: image
[148] Holland PC. CS–US interval as a determinant of the form of
and action. Cognition 1990;37:105–31.
Pavlovian appetitive conditioned responses. J Exp Psychol Anim
[136] Colwill RM, Rescorla RA. Associative structures in instrumental
learning. In: Bower GH, editor. The psychology of learning and
[149] Vandercar DH, Schneiderman N. Interstimulus interval functions in
motivation, vol. 20. New York: Academic Press; 1986. p. 55–104.
different response systems during classical discrimination condition-
[137] Dickinson A. Expectancy theory in animal conditioning. In:
ing in rabbits. Psychon Sci 1967;9:9–10.
Klein SB, Mowrer RR, editors. Contemporary learning theories:
[150] Wilkie DM, Spetch ML. Pigeon’s delayed matching to sample errors
Pavlovian conditioning and the status of traditional learning theory.
are not always due to forgetting. Behav Anal Lett 1981;1:317–23.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1989. p. 279–308.
[151] Roberts WA, Grant DS. Interaction of sample and comparison
[138] Adams CD. Variations in the sensitivity of instrumental responding
stimuli in delayed matching to sample with the pigeon. J Exp
to reinforcer devaluation. Q J Exp Psychol B 1982;34:77–98.
Psychol Anim Behav Process 1978;4:68–82.
[139] Colwill RM, Rescorla RA. The role of response reinforcer
[152] Roitblat HL, Harley HE. Spatial delayed matching-to-sample
associations increases throughout extended instrumental training.
performance by rats: learning, memory, and proactive interference.
J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 1988;14:71–82.
[140] Balleine BW. Incentive processes in instrumental conditioning. In:
[153] Timberlake W. Motivational modes in behavior systems. In:
Mowrer RR, Klein SB, editors. Handbook of contemporary learning
Mowrer RR, Klein SB, editors. Handbook of contemporary learning
theories. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 2001. p. 307–66.
theories. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 2001. p. 155–209.
[141] Dickinson A, Balleine B. Motivational control of goal-directed
[154] Odling-Smee FJ. Background stimuli and the inter-stimulus interval
action. Anim Learn Behav 1994;22:1–18.
during Pavlovian conditioning. Q J Exp Psychol A 1975;27:387–92.
SICHERHEITSDATENBLATTgemäß Verordnung (EG) Nr. 1907/2006 (REACH) Art. 2280, INSEKT-EX 1. Bezeichnung des Stoffes bzw. des Gemischs und des Unternehmens Produktidentifikator Bezeichnung / Handelsname: REACH Registrierungsnr.: Relevante identifizierte Verwendungen des Stoffs oder Gemischs und Verwendungen, von denen abgeraten wird Verwendung des Stoffes / des Gemisches: Ein
Mobilgrease XHP 220 serie smeervetten zijn lithium-complex smeervetten bedoeld voor een verlengde service met een breed toepassingsgebied en in uiteenlopende bedrijfsomstandigheden. Deze smeervetten overtreffen conventionele smeervetten door het gebruik van onze gepatenteerde toonaangevende lithium-complex productietechnologie. Deze smeervetten zijn ontwikkeld om uitmuntende hoge temperatuur pres